题目内容 (请给出正确答案)
提问人:网友z*****n 发布时间:2022年4月19日 00:08
[判断题]

国际游资由于其投机性很大,因此其对市场并没有积极的成份

参考答案
十点题库官方参考答案 (由十点题库聘请的专业题库老师提供的解答)
更多“国际游资由于其投机性很大,因此其对市场并没有积极的成份”相关的问题
下列选项中错误的是______
A.自身免疫性溶血性贫血——抗人球蛋白试验阳性
B.PNH——酸溶血试验阳性
C.PK酶缺陷症——自溶试验增高,加葡萄糖不能纠正
D.β-珠蛋白生成障碍性贫血——血红蛋白电泳,HbA2减少
E.慢性血管内溶血——尿含铁血黄素试验阳性
点击查看答案
算备付金账户日终清算后,余额低于规定上存比例或额度时,需在()补足。
A.下1个工作日内
B.下2个工作日内
C.3个工作日内
D.随时
点击查看答案
慢性胆囊炎的CT特征性表现是()
A.胆囊大,囊壁水肿,密度低
B.胆囊正常大小,肝内胆管扩张
C.胆囊小,囊壁增厚
D.胆囊大,胆总管扩张
E.胆囊小,囊壁正常
点击查看答案
治疗强迫症的心理治疗方法包括()
A.支持性心理治疗B.行为治疗C.精神分析治疗D.生物反馈治疗E.催眠治疗

B.行为治疗C.精神分析治疗D.生物反馈治疗E.催眠治疗

C.精神分析治疗D.生物反馈治疗E.催眠治疗

D.生物反馈治疗E.催眠治疗

E.催眠治疗
点击查看答案
发生脱位率最高的关节是()
点击查看答案
在进行α=0.05之右尾检定时,若检定统计量Z=1.15,则是否推翻虚无假设H0?
点击查看答案
以砾石作填料时,分层夯实时其最大粒径不宜大于()。
\tA.200mm
\tB.300mm
\tC.100mm
\tD.400mm
点击查看答案
新生儿肺炎的护理诊断包括(    )    A.清理呼吸道无效    B.气体交换受损    C.体温调节无效    D.营养失调    E.潜在并发症:心力衰竭
A.清理呼吸道无效B.气体交换受损C.体温调节无效D.营养失调
点击查看答案
若信号x(t)为实奇函数,则其频谱X(f)将是 函数。
A. 实偶B. 虚偶C. 实奇D. 虚奇
点击查看答案
信息系统的基本功能包括()。
A . 信息采集
B . 信息输出
C . 信息处理
D . 信息存储
E . 信息传输
点击查看答案
对建造目标的危险性的正式估计属于()
A.可行性研究阶段的HSE管理 B.设计阶段的HSE管理发包 C.和约阶段的HSE管理 D.施工中的HSE管理
点击查看答案
The mid-sixties saw the start of a project that, along with other similar research, was to teach us a great deal about the chimpanzee mind. This was Project Washoe, conceived by Trixie and Allen Gardner. They purchased an infant chimpanzee and began to teach her the signs of ASL, the American Sign Language used by the deaf. Twenty years earlier another husband and wife team, Richard and Cathy Hayes, had tried, with an almost total lack of success, to teach a young chimp, Vikki, to talk. The Hayes*s undertaking taught us a lot about the chimpanzee mind, but Vikki, although she did well in IQ tests, and was clearly an intelligent youngster, could not learn human speech. The Gardners, however, achieved spectacular success with their pupil, Washoe. Not only did she learn signs easily, but she quickly began to string them together in meaningful ways. It was clear that each sign evoked, in her mind, a mental image of the object it represented. If, for example, she was asked, in sign language, to fetch an apple, she would go and locate an apple that was out of sight in another room. Other chimps entered the project, some starting their lives in deaf signing families before joining Washoe. And finally Washoe adopted an infant, Loulis. He came from a lab where no thought of teaching signs had ever penetrated. When he was with Washoe he was given no lessons in language acquisition—not by humans, anyway. Yet by the time he was eight years old he had made fifty-eight signs in their correct contexts. How did he learn them Mostly, it seems, by imitating the behavior of Washoe and the other three signing chimps, Dar, Moja and Tam. Sometimes, though, he received tuition from Washoe herself. One day, for example, she began to swagger about bipedally, hair bristling, signing food! food! food! in great excitement. She had seen a human approaching with a bar of chocolate. Loulis, only eighteen months old, watched passively. Suddenly Washoe stopped her swaggering, went over to him, took his hand, and moulded the sign for food (fingers pointing towards mouth). Another time, in a similar context,, she made the sign for chewing gum—but with her hand on his body. On a third occasion Washoe picked up a small chair, took it over to Loulis, set it down in front of him, and very distinctly made the chair sign three times, watching him closely as she did so. The two food signs became incorporated into Loulis’s vocabulary but the sign for chair did not. Obviously the priorities of a young chimp are similar to those of a human child! Chimpanzees who have been taught a language can combine signs creatively in order to describe objects for which they have no symbol. Washoe, for example, puzzled her caretakers by asking, repeatedly, for a rock berry. Eventually it transpired that she was referring to brazil nuts which she had encountered for the first time a while before. Another language-trained chimp described a cucumber as a green banana. They can even invent signs. Lucy, as she got older, had to be put on a leash for her outings. One day, eager to set off but having no sign for leash, she signaled her wishes by holding a crooked index finger to the ring on her collar. This sign became part of her vocabulary.
The mid-sixties saw the start of a project that, along with other similar research, was to teach us a great deal about the chimpanzee mind. This was Project Washoe, conceived by Trixie and Allen Gardner. They purchased an infant chimpanzee and began to teach her the signs of ASL, the American Sign Language used by the deaf. Twenty years earlier another husband and wife team, Richard and Cathy Hayes, had tried, with an almost total lack of success, to teach a young chimp, Vikki, to talk. The Hayes*s undertaking taught us a lot about the chimpanzee mind, but Vikki, although she did well in IQ tests, and was clearly an intelligent youngster, could not learn human speech. The Gardners, however, achieved spectacular success with their pupil, Washoe. Not only did she learn signs easily, but she quickly began to string them together in meaningful ways. It was clear that each sign evoked, in her mind, a mental image of the object it represented. If, for example, she was asked, in sign language, to fetch an apple, she would go and locate an apple that was out of sight in another room.
Other chimps entered the project, some starting their lives in deaf signing families before joining Washoe. And finally Washoe adopted an infant, Loulis. He came from a lab where no thought of teaching signs had ever penetrated. When he was with Washoe he was given no lessons in language acquisition—not by humans, anyway. Yet by the time he was eight years old he had made fifty-eight signs in their correct contexts. How did he learn them Mostly, it seems, by imitating the behavior of Washoe and the other three signing chimps, Dar, Moja and Tam. Sometimes, though, he received tuition from Washoe herself. One day, for example, she began to swagger about bipedally, hair bristling, signing food! food! food! in great excitement. She had seen a human approaching with a bar of chocolate. Loulis, only eighteen months old, watched passively. Suddenly Washoe stopped her swaggering, went over to him, took his hand, and moulded the sign for food (fingers pointing towards mouth). Another time, in a similar context,, she made the sign for chewing gum—but with her hand on his body. On a third occasion Washoe picked up a small chair, took it over to Loulis, set it down in front of him, and very distinctly made the chair sign three times, watching him closely as she did so. The two food signs became incorporated into Loulis’s vocabulary but the sign for chair did not. Obviously the priorities of a young chimp are similar to those of a human child!
Chimpanzees who have been taught a language can combine signs creatively in order to describe objects for which they have no symbol. Washoe, for example, puzzled her caretakers by asking, repeatedly, for a rock berry. Eventually it transpired that she was referring to brazil nuts which she had encountered for the first time a while before. Another language-trained chimp described a cucumber as a green banana. They can even invent signs. Lucy, as she got older, had to be put on a leash for her outings. One day, eager to set off but having no sign for leash, she signaled her wishes by holding a crooked index finger to the ring on her collar. This sign became part of her vocabulary.
点击查看答案
寄生虫检测中,不属于SPF级豚鼠必须检测项目的是()。
A.卡氏肺孢子虫
B.全部蠕虫
C.鞭毛虫
D.纤毛虫
点击查看答案
《条例》制定是根据《中国共产党章程》、《中华人民共和国宪法》和有关法律。()
点击查看答案
Can New Doctors Be Harmflul to Your Health Between the nerves, the unfamiliarity and the urge to impress, few people do themselves justice on the first day of a new job. When it comes to doctors starting out in emergency medicine, though, are patients’ lives being put at risk According to research from Imperial College London, the death rate among patients admitted to English hospitals on the first Wednesday in August -the day, traditionally, that newly graduated doctors take up their posts--was, on average, 6% higher than for those admitted the last Wednesday in July. An influx of new medical staff, in other words, just might be very, very bad for your health. In the university’ s study (the biggest of its kind so far), published Sept. 23 in PLoS One, the online scientific journal of the U. S. Public Library of Science, scientists analyzed close to 300,000 patients admitted to state - run hospitals across England on those two Wednesdays from 2000 to 2008. The health of the patients, who were split evenly between the July and August admission days, was tracked for a week. While there was little difference between the crude death rates for each seven -day period, when researchers controlled for the patients’ age, sex, socioeconomic status and secondary medical problems, the odds of dying was found to be 6% higher for those admired on the Wednesday in August. Among so - called medical achnissions - all those not suffering from cancer or requiring surgery -the odds were 8% higher. Should patients be nervous Scientists have for years been examining the patient risk associated with a change over of medical staff Smaller studies conducted over the past two decades in Britain and the U. S. -where researchers label it the "July phenomenon," after the month in which medical students usually begin training--have often proved inconelusive. Even the "small but significant" discrepancy highlighted by Imperial’ s study doesn’t point to a particular cause. It’ s possible that patients admitted on the days junior doctors began work were simply in worse health than those taken in the week earlier. Some hospitals may have been more reluctant to admit patients with less - serious problems on the days new staff started work, limiting the number of cases young medics had to deal with but increasing the concentration of acutely ill patients in the process. "So it may not necessarily be directly related to the quality of care, "says Paul Aylin, an epidemiologist at Imperial College London and senior author of the study.
Between the nerves, the unfamiliarity and the urge to impress, few people do themselves justice on the first day of a new job. When it comes to doctors starting out in emergency medicine, though, are patients’ lives being put at risk According to research from Imperial College London, the death rate among patients admitted to English hospitals on the first Wednesday in August -the day, traditionally, that newly graduated doctors take up their posts--was, on average, 6% higher than for those admitted the last Wednesday in July. An influx of new medical staff, in other words, just might be very, very bad for your health.
In the university’ s study (the biggest of its kind so far), published Sept. 23 in PLoS One, the online scientific journal of the U. S. Public Library of Science, scientists analyzed close to 300,000 patients admitted to state - run hospitals across England on those two Wednesdays from 2000 to 2008. The health of the patients, who were split evenly between the July and August admission days, was tracked for a week. While there was little difference between the crude death rates for each seven -day period, when researchers controlled for the patients’ age, sex, socioeconomic status and secondary medical problems, the odds of dying was found to be 6% higher for those admired on the Wednesday in August. Among so - called medical achnissions - all those not suffering from cancer or requiring surgery -the odds were 8% higher. Should patients be nervous Scientists have for years been examining the patient risk associated with a change over of medical staff Smaller studies conducted over the past two decades in Britain and the U. S. -where researchers label it the "July phenomenon," after the month in which medical students usually begin training--have often proved inconelusive.
Even the "small but significant" discrepancy highlighted by Imperial’ s study doesn’t point to a particular cause. It’ s possible that patients admitted on the days junior doctors began work were simply in worse health than those taken in the week earlier. Some hospitals may have been more reluctant to admit patients with less - serious problems on the days new staff started work, limiting the number of cases young medics had to deal with but increasing the concentration of acutely ill patients in the process. "So it may not necessarily be directly related to the quality of care, "says Paul Aylin, an epidemiologist at Imperial College London and senior author of the study.
点击查看答案
客服
TOP